Bottlenose_dolphin by zzzmarcus

VIEWS: 421 PAGES: 14

									From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Bottlenose dolphin

Bottlenose dolphin
Bottlenose dolphins

Bottlenose dolphin breaching in the bow wave of a boat

Size comparison against an average human

Scientific classification Kingdom: Phylum: Class: Order: Family: Genus: Animalia Chordata Mammalia Cetacea Delphinidae Tursiops
Gervais 1855

Bottlenose dolphin range (in blue)

Species See text

dolphins, but group size varies from solitary bottlenose dolphins up to groups of over 100 or even occasionally over 1000 animals. Their diet consists mainly of forage fish. Dolphin groups often work as a team to harvest schools of fish, but they also hunt individually. Dolphins search for prey primarily using echolocation, which is similar to sonar. They emit clicking sounds and listen for the return echo to determine the location and shape of nearby items, including potential prey. Bottlenose dolphins also use sound for communication. Sounds used for communication include squeaks and whistles emitted from the blowhole and sounds emitted through body language, such as leaping from the water and slapping their tails on the water. There have been numerous investigations of bottlenose dolphin intelligence. Such testing has included tests of mimicry, use of artificial language, object categorization and self-recognition. This intelligence has driven considerable interaction with humans. Bottlenose dolphins are popular from aquarium shows and television programs such as Flipper. They have also been trained by militaries for tasks such as locating sea mines or detecting and marking enemy divers. In some areas they cooperate with local fishermen by driving fish towards the fishermen and eating the fish that escape the fishermen’s nets. Some encounters with humans are harmful to the dolphins: people hunt bottlenose dolphins for food, and dolphins are killed inadvertently as a bycatch of tuna fishing.

Bottlenose dolphins, the genus Tursiops, are the most common and well-known members of the family Delphinidae, the family of oceanic dolphins. Recent molecular studies show the genus contains two species, the Common Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) and the Indo-Pacific Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops aduncus), where previous thought was that this was one species. Bottlenose dolphins inhabit warm and temperate seas worldwide. Bottlenose dolphins live in groups called pods that typically number about 15

Taxonomy
Scientists have long been aware that Tursiops might consist of more than one species. The advent of molecular genetics has allowed much greater insight into this previously intractable problem. The consensus amongst scientists is that there are two species:[1] • the Common Bottlenose Dolphin (T. truncatus), found in most tropical to temperate oceans; colour is grey, with the shade of grey varying among populations; can be bluish-grey, brownish-grey, or even nearly black; often darker on the back

1

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Bottlenose dolphin
Some recent genetic evidence suggests that the Indo-Pacific Bottlenase belongs in the genus Stenella, it being more like the Atlantic Spotted Dolphin (Stenella frontalis) than the Common Bottlenose.[9][10]

Indo-Pacific Bottlenose Dolphin, T. aduncus from the rostrum to behind the dorsal fin,[2] and • the Indo-Pacific Bottlenose Dolphin (T. aduncus), living in the waters around India, northern Australia, South China, the Red Sea, and the eastern coast of Africa; back is dark-grey and belly is lighter grey or nearly white with grey spots.[3] The following are sometimes also recognized as subspecies of T. truncatus: • the Pacific Bottlenose Dolphin (T. gillii or T. truncatus gillii), living in the Pacific; has a black line from the eye to the forehead [4][5] • the Black Sea Bottlenose Dolphin (T. truncatus ponticus), living in the Black Sea.[4][6] There are two ecotypes of the Common Bottlenose Dolphin within the Western North Atlantic.[7] These are represented by the shallower water or coastal ecotype and the more offshore ecotype.[7] The ranges of these ecotypes overlap, but they have been shown to be genetically distinct.[7] However, they are not currently described as separate species or subspecies. In general, there is significant genetic variation between populations of Common Bottlenose Dolphin, even among nearby populations.[2] As a result of this genetic variation, it is possible that there are several distinct undescribed species currently considered as populations of Common Bottlenose Dolphin.[2] Much of the old scientific data in the field combine data about the two species into a single group, making it effectively useless in determining the structural differences between the two species. The IUCN lists both species as data deficient in their Red List of endangered species because of this issue.[8]

Wolphin Kawili’Kai at the Sea Life Park in Hawaii.

Hybrids
The bottlenose dolphins have been known to hybridize with a number of other dolphin species. Hybrids between Risso’s Dolphin and bottlenose dolphins occur both in the wild and in captivity.[11][12] The best known hybrid is the Wolphin, a False Killer Whale-Bottlenose Dolphin hybrid. The Wolphin is a fertile hybrid, and two such Wolphins currently live at the Sea Life Park in Hawaii, the first having been born in 1985 from a male False Killer Whale and a female Bottlenose. Wolphins have also been observed in the wild.[13] In captivity, a Bottlenose Dolphin and a Rough-toothed Dolphin produced hybrid offspring.[14] A Common Dolphin-Bottlenose Dolphin hybrid that was born in captivity lives at SeaWorld California.[15][16] Various other dolphin hybrids live in captivity around the world or have been reported the wild, such as a Bottlenose Dolphin-Atlantic Spotted Dolphin hybrid.[17]

Behavior
The bottlenose dolphin normally lives in small groups, usually containing up to 15 animals.[2] However, group size may be highly variable since they live in fission-fusion societies within which individuals associate in small groups that change in composition,

2

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Bottlenose dolphin
similar to behaviour they show towards injured members of their own species.[22] In November 2004, a more dramatic report of dolphin intervention came from New Zealand. Four lifeguards, swimming 100 m (328 ft) off the coast near Whangarei, were approached by a shark (reportedly a Great White Shark). A group of bottlenose dolphins, most likely sensing danger to the swimmers, herded them together and tightly surrounded them for forty minutes, preventing an attack from the shark, as they returned to shore.[23] Dolphins have also been documented exhibiting altruistic behaviour toward other sea creatures. On Mahia Beach, New Zealand on March 10, 2008[24] two Pygmy Sperm Whales — a female and calf — became stranded on the beach. Rescuers, including Department of Conservation officer Malcolm Smith, attempted to refloat the whales, however their efforts failed four times. Shortly before the whales were to be euthanized a playful bottlenose dolphin known to local residents as Moko arrived and, after seemingly communicating with the whales, led them 200 meters along a sandbar to the open sea.[25] The bottlenose dolphin is a predator however, and it also often shows aggressive behaviour. This includes fights among males for rank and access to females, as well as aggression towards sharks, certain Orcas, and other smaller species of dolphins. During the mating season male dolphins compete vigorously with each other through displays of toughness and size with a series of acts such as head-butting. At least one population, off Scotland, has been observed to practice infanticide, and has also been filmed attacking and killing Harbour Porpoises. Researchers at the University of Aberdeen in Scotland have discovered that the local bottlenose dolphins attack and kill Harbour Porpoises without eating them due to competition for a decreasing food supply.[26] The bottlenose dolphin sometimes forms mixed species groups with certain other species from the dolphin family, particularly larger species such as the Short-finned Pilot Whale, the False Killer Whale and Risso’s Dolphin.[27][28][29] Interactions with smaller species, such as the Atlantic Spotted Dolphin and the Rough-toothed Dolphin, also occur in wild.[27][30] While interactions with smaller species are sometimes afffiliative, they can also be aggressive.[27]

An adult female bottlenose dolphin with her young, Moray Firth, Scotland

A bottlenose dolphin attacks and kills a Harbour Porpoise at Chanonry Point, Scotland often on a daily or hourly basis.[18][19] Typically, a group of adult females and their young live together in a pod, and juveniles in a mixed pod. Several of these pods can join together to form larger groups of 100 dolphins or more. These groups can occasionally exceed 1000 dolphins.[2] Males live mostly alone or in groups of 2-3 and join the pods for short periods of time. Bottlenose dolphins studied by researchers of the Bottlenose Dolphin Research Institute BDRI off the island of Sardinia show non-random social behaviour during feeding activities and their social behaviour differs depending on the feeding activity in which they are engaged.[20] In Sardinia, the presence of a floating marine fin-fish farm has been linked to a change in bottlenose dolphin distribution as a result of high fish density around the floating cages in the farming area.[21] The species sometimes shows curiosity towards humans in or near water. Occasionally, bottlenose dolphins have rescued injured divers by raising them to the surface. This is

3

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Bottlenose dolphin
interest is approached the echo grows louder, and the dolphins adjust by decreasing the intensity of the emitted sounds. (This is in contrast to the technique used by bat echolocation and artificial sonar where the sensitivity of the sound receptor is attenuated.) As the animal approaches the target the interclick interval also decreases, as each click is usually produced after the round-trip travel time of the previous click is completed. Details of the dolphin’s echolocation, such as signal strength, spectral qualities, and discrimination abilities have been well-investigated by researchers.[37] Bottlenose dolphins are able to extract shape information from their echolocative sense, suggesting that they are able to form an "echoic image" of their targets.[38] Dolphins also have sharp eyesight. The eyes are located at the sides of the head and have a tapetum lucidum, or reflecting membrane at the back of the retina, which aids vision in dim light. Their horseshoe-shaped double-slit pupil enables the dolphin to have good vision both in air and underwater, despite the different densities of these media.[39] When underwater the eyeball’s lens serves to focus light, whereas in the in-air environment the typically bright light serves to contract the specialized pupil, resulting in sharpness from a smaller aperture (similar to a pinhole camera). By contrast their sense of smell is poor,[40] as would be expected since the blowhole, the analogue to the nose, is closed in the underwater environment, and opens only voluntarily for breathing. The olfactory nerves as well as the olfactory lobe in the brain are missing.[40] Bottlenose dolphins are able to detect salty, sweet, bitter (quinine sulphate), and sour (citric acid) tastes, but this has not been well-studied.[40] Anecdotally, some animals in captivity have been noted to have preferences for food fish types although it is not clear that this preference is mediated by taste.[40] Bottlenose dolphins communicate with one another through squeaks, whistles, and body language. Examples of body language include leaping out of the water, snapping jaws, slapping tails on the surface of the water, and butting heads with one another. All of these gestures are a way for the dolphins to convey messages.[41] The sounds and gestures that bottlenose dolphins produce help keep track of other dolphins in the group and

Diet

Tuna are among the bottlenose dolphin’s preferred foods The bottlenose dolphin’s diet consists mainly of small fish and squid.[31] Although the diet varies by location, certain preferences are shared among many population. Such preferences include fish from the mullet family, the tuna and mackerel family, and the drum and croaker family.[31] Its cone-like teeth serve to grasp but not to chew food.[32][33] When a shoal of fish is found dolphins work as a team to keep the fish close together and move them towards the shore in order to maximize the harvest.[2] They also search for fish alone, often bottom dwelling species. The bottlenose dolphin sometimes uses a strategy called "fish whacking", in which a fish is stunned and sometimes thrown out of the water with the fluke to make it easier to catch and eat.[34][35] Perceived conflict of interactions between bottlenose dolphins and coastal, small scale commercial fisheries has been reported in a number of Mediterranean areas. Common Bottlenose Dolphins are probably attracted to fishing net activities because they make it easier for the dolphins to exploit a concentrated food source.[36]

Senses and communication
The dolphin’s search for food is aided by a form of echolocation similar to sonar: they locate objects by producing sounds and listening for the echo. A broadband burst pulse of clicking sounds is emitted in a focused beam in front of the dolphin. To hear the returning echo they have two small ear openings behind the eyes but most sound waves are transmitted to the inner ear through the lower jaw. As the object of

4

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
alert other dolphins to possible dangers and nearby food. They produce sounds using six air sacs near their blow hole (they lack vocal cords). Each animal has a characteristic frequency-modulated narrow-band signature vocalization (signature whistle) which is uniquely identifying.[42] Other communication uses about 30 distinguishable sounds, and although famously proposed by John Lilly in the 1950s, a "dolphin language" has not been found. However, Herman, Richards, & Wolz demonstrated the comprehension of an artificial language by two bottlenose dolphins (named Akeakamai and Phoenix) in the period of skepticism toward animal language following Herbert Terrace’s critique.[43]

Bottlenose dolphin

Respiration and Sleep
The bottlenose dolphin has a single blowhole located on the dorsal surface of the head consisting of a hole and a muscular flap. The flap is closed during muscle relaxation and opens during contraction.[44] A bottlenose dolphin can store almost twice as much oxygen in proportion to its body weight as a human can. The dolphin can store 36 millilitres of oxygen per kilogram of body weight, compared with 20 millilitres per kilogram for humans.[45] This is an adaptation to diving.[45] The bottlenose dolphin typically rises to the surface to breathe through its blowhole 2-3 times per minute;[46] if necessary, it has the ability to remain submerged for up to 20 minutes.[47] As a direct result of the voluntary breathing requirement scientists have determined that during the sleeping cycle one brain hemisphere remains active while the other hemisphere shuts down.[48] The sleeping cycle lasts for approximately 8 hours during each 24 hour period, in increments of several minutes (or less) to several hours. During the sleeping cycle dolphins remain near the surface swimming slowly or "logging", occasionally closing one eye.[49]

Mother and juvenile bottlenose dolphins head to the seafloor. females to prevent access from other males.[27][52] Mating occurs with the male and female in contact belly to belly.[52] Bottlenose males have large testes in relation to their body size, and fight for their females. Bottlenose dolphins can produce highly concentrated sperm, but only during the mating season. The rest of the year sperm production drops markedly, suggesting that the production of sperm is energetically expensive, so dolphins don’t waste it during times when sex is mostly social. The size of the testes can change, becoming the largest during the breeding season. Seasonal peaks in male testosterone level, sperm concentration, and a high testis to body mass ratio suggest sperm competition. The bigger the testes, the more sperm an individual can hold, allowing a male to wash out the sperm of the previous male that mated with the same female and still leave some of his sperm for fertilization. [53] The average gestation period is 12 months.[2] Births can occur at any time of year, although peak births occur in warmer months.[31] The young are born in shallow

Reproduction
The male has a slit on the underside of its body into which the penis retracts and is concealed.[50] The female has one genital slit, housing the vagina and the anus. A mammary slit is positioned on either side of the female’s genital slit.[51] Males compete for access to females. Such competition can take the form of physical competition or the form of herding

5

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
water, sometimes assisted by a "midwife" (which may be male). A single calf is born,[54] about 1 m (3 ft) long at birth.[2] To speed up the nursing process, the mother can eject milk from her mammary glands. There are two slits, one on either side of the genital slit, each housing one nipple.[51] The calf is nursed for 18 to 20 months, and continues to associate closely with its mother for several years after weaning.[55] Females become sexually mature between age 5 and 13, males a little later, between age 9 and 14.[31] Females reproduce every 2 to 6 years.[2] Janet Mann, a professor of biology and psychology at Georgetown University, argues that the strong personal behaviour among male dolphin calves is about bond formation and benefits the species in an evolutionary context. She cites studies showing that these dolphins later in life as adults are inseparable, and the male bonds forged earlier in life work together for protection as well as locating females to reproduce with.[56] Male bottlenose dolphins have been observed working in pairs or larger groups to follow and/or restrict the movement of a female for weeks at a time, waiting for her to become sexually receptive.[27]

Bottlenose dolphin
• acoustic and behavioral mimicry[57][58] • comprehension of novel sequences in an artificial language[59][60] • memory[61] • monitoring of self behaviours[62] • discrimination and matching[61][63] • comprehension of symbols for various body parts[64] • comprehension of the pointing gesture and gaze (as made by dolphins or humans)[65] • mirror self-recognition[66][67] Recent research has shown that bottlenose dolphins are capable of comprehending numerical values. In an experiment where a dolphin was shown two panels with a various number of dots of different size and position, the dolphin was able to touch the panel with a greater number of dots.[68]

Tool use and culture
In 1997, tool use was described in bottlenose dolphins in Shark Bay. A dolphin will stick a marine sponge on its rostrum, presumably to protect it when searching for food in the sandy sea bottom.[69] The behaviour has only been observed in this bay, and is almost exclusively shown by females. This is the only known case of tool use in marine mammals outside of Sea Otters. An elaborate study in 2005 showed that mothers most likely teach the behaviour to their daughters.[70] Subsets of populations in Mauritania are known to engage in interspecific cooperative fishing with human fishermen. The dolphins drive a school of fish towards the shore where humans await with their nets. In the confusion of casting nets, the dolphins catch a large number of fish as well. Intraspecific cooperative foraging techniques have also been observed, and some propose that these behaviours are transmitted through cultural means. Rendell & Whitehead have proposed a structure for the study of culture in cetaceans,[71] although this view has been controversial.[72] Cultural or learned behaviour has occurred on the south Australian coast, near Adelaide. Three of the bottlenose dolphins in the area have been recorded ’tail-walking’, whereby they hold the upper part of their bodies vertically out of the water, and move along the surface with movements of their tail. This is rarely seen in the wild, being much more commonly seen as a trained

Intelligence
See also: Cetacean intelligence

Bottlenose dolphin responding to human hand gestures

Cognition
Cognitive abilities investigated in the dolphin include concept formation, sensory skills, and the use of mental representation of dolphins. Such research has been ongoing from the 1970s. This includes:-

6

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
behaviour in dolphinariums. In the 1980s, one of the females in the local population was kept at a local dolphinarium for three weeks, and the scientist suggests, she copied the tail-walking behaviour from other dolphins. Two other wild adult female dolphins have now copied it from her.[73]

Bottlenose dolphin
estimated to consist of around 150 animals and is declining by around 6% per year due to the impact of harassment and traumatic death, water pollution and reduction in food availability. Less local climate change such as increasing water temperature may also play a role.[79] In U.S. waters, hunting and harassing of marine mammals is forbidden in almost all circumstances. The international trade in dolphins is also tightly controlled. The man-made chemical perfluorooctanesulfonic acid (PFOS) may be compromising the immune system of bottlenose dolphins.[80] PFOS was found to affect the immune system of male mice at a concentration of 91.5 ppb,[81] while PFOS has been reported in bottlenose dolphins in excess of 1000 ppb.[82] High levels of metal contaminants have been measured in bottlenose dolphin tissues in many areas of the globe. A recent study found very high levels of cadmium and mercury in bottlenose dolphins from South Australia.[83] The high metal levels recorded in South Australian dolphins were later found to be associated with kidney malformations, indicating possible health effects of high metal concentrations in dolphins.[84]

Cortical neurons
Some researchers have theorized that mammal intelligence is correlated to the number of nerve cells (neurons) in the cortex of the brain.[74] Bottlenose dolphins have about 5.8 billion cortical neurons, fewer than humans (who have 11.5 billion) and somewhat fewer than Chimpanzees (which have 6.2 billion), but more than Gorillas, which have 4.3 billion cortical neurons and are often considered to be very intelligent animals.[74]

Natural predators
The bottlenose dolphin has few natural predators. But some large shark species, such as the tiger shark, the dusky shark, the great white shark and the bull shark can prey on the bottlenose dolphin, especially the young.[22][52][75][76][77] However, the bottlenose dolphin is capable of defending itself by charging at the shark; indeed, dolphin ’mobbing’ behaviour of sharks can occasionally prove fatal for the shark.[22] Even a single adult dolphin is dangerous prey for a shark of similar size. Certain (but not all) orca populations may also prey on dolphins, but this seems rare.[22] While certain orcas that eat other mammals prey on the dolphin, other non-mammal eating orcas have been seen swimming with dolphins. Swimming in pods allows dolphins to better defend themselves against predators. Bottlenose dolphins either use complex evasive strategies to outswim their predators or they will batter the predator to death. Bottlenose dolphins will also aid injured dolphins by holding them above water for air.[78]

Interaction with humans

Conservation
Bottlenose dolphins are not endangered. Their future is stable because of their abundance and high adaptability. However, some specific populations are threatened due to various environmental changes. The population in the Moray Firth in Scotland is

K-Dog, trained by the US Navy to find mines and boobytraps underwater, leaping out of the water

7

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Bottlenose dolphins are still occasionally killed in dolphin drive hunts for their meat or because they compete for fish.[85] Bottlenose dolphins (and several other dolphin species) often travel together with tuna, and can get caught in the nets used to catch the tuna, which can result in the death of the dolphins.[86][87] This has led to boycotts of tuna products and a "dolphin-safe" label for tuna caught with methods that do not endanger dolphins.[87] Bottlenose dolphins living around coastal regions have received much attention due to their increased vulnerability of inhabiting areas where marine traffic is concentrated. Solitary wild bottlenose dolphins and man frequenting the same small areas makes boat interaction more or less inevitable. Researchers of the Bottlenose Dolphin Research Institute BDRI provided the first quantified data about solitary bottlenose dolphin diving behaviour in the presence and absence of boats [88]. By examining the influence of different types of vessel it was evident that the dolphin elicited a stronger response to tourist than fisheries vessels. The behaviour vessels display around the dolphin as well as speed, engine type and distance of approach are all factors that needed to be taken into consideration when analysing the behavioural changes observed. Bottlenose dolphins are often seen performing in dolphin shows. These shows have generated controversy. Some animal welfare activists object to these shows on the basis that dolphins do not have adequate space or receive adequate care or stimulation. However, others support the shows on the basis that the dolphins are properly cared for and enjoy interacting with humans.[22][89] Eight bottlenose dolphins that lived in captivity at the Marine Life Aquarium in Gulfport, Mississippi were swept away from their aquarium pool during Hurricane Katrina in August 2005. They were later found alive in the Gulf of Mexico and rescued.[90] Certain therapies for handicapped children can include interactions with bottlenose dolphins.[22] The military of the United States and Russia train bottlenose dolphins as military dolphins for wartime tasks such as locating sea mines and detecting enemy divers.[91][92] The USA’s program is the U.S. Navy Marine Mammal Program, located in San Diego, California.[93] Bottlenose dolphins are difficult to

Bottlenose dolphin
detect with radar and sonar. Even when detected by an enemy, a dolphin is likely to be dismissed as a harmless sea creature. Navy scientist fit the dolphins with equipment that amplify their natural sonar pulses, and relay the information back to Navy Intelligence. The equipment strengthens the dolphin’s sonar burst; giving the dolphin enhanced scouting abilities, and can even allow it to inflict damage on medium density materials such as low-grade steel, carbon-plating, and wood. This has proven particularly effective at detecting, and disabling underwater intruders including small drones, mines, divers and even ships and submarines that use naval stealth technology.[94] There are commercial dolphin encounter enterprises and tours around the world. In addition to such endeavors, a common close interaction between humans and dolphins occurs when the animals swim with and surface near surfers at the beach.[95] In the town of Laguna in south Brazil, a pod of bottlenose dolphins is known to drive fish towards fishermen who stand at the beach in shallow waters. One dolphin will then roll over, which the fishermen take as a sign to throw out their nets. The dolphins feed on the escaping fish. The dolphins were not trained for this behaviour; the collaboration has been going on at least since 1847. Similar cooperative fisheries also exist in Mauritania, Africa.[96]

In popular culture
The popular television show Flipper, created by Ivan Tors, portrayed a bottlenose dolphin[97] in a friendly relationship with two boys, Sandy and Bud. A kind of seagoing Lassie, Flipper understood English unusually well and was a marked hero: "Go tell Dad we’re in trouble, Flipper! Hurry!" The show’s theme song contains the lyric no one you see / is smarter than he. The television show was based on a 1963 film, and remade as a feature film in 1996 starring Elijah Wood and Paul Hogan, as well as a television series running from 1995-2000 starring Jessica Alba.[98] Other television appearances by bottlenose dolphins include seaQuest DSV, and the HBO TV movie Zeus and Roxanne, in which a female bottlenose dolphin befriends a male dog.

8

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
The NFL franchise, the Miami Dolphins use the bottlenose Dolphin are their mascot and team logo Bottlenose dolphins also have appeared in several novels. In The Hitchhiker’s Guide to the Galaxy and one of its sequels, So Long, and Thanks For All the Fish, the dolphins try to warn humans of the impending destruction of earth but their behaviour was misinterpreted as playful acrobatics. Bottlenose dolphins are also central to David Brin’s series of Uplift Universe novels, particularly Startide Rising, where they are one of the two non-human species (along with Chimpanzees) to have been uplifted to sentience. Bottlenose dolphins are also primary characters in Anne McCaffrey’s "Dragonriders of Pern" series, especially "The Dolphins of Pern". Bottlenose dolphins also are incorporated into the science fiction video game series Ecco the Dolphin. A dolphin, Delphineus, is also featured in the video game EchoQuest: The Search for Cetus, assisting the boy, Adam, in finding the sea king Cetus (a sperm whale), as well as assisting in cleaning up the underwater environment where he lives. Factual descriptions of the dolphins date back into antiquity - the writings of Aristotle, Oppian and Pliny the Elder all mention the species.[31][99]

Bottlenose dolphin
ed. Mammal Species of the World (3rd edition ed.). Johns Hopkins University Press. pp. 723–743. ISBN 0-801-88221-4. http://www.bucknell.edu/msw3/ browse.asp?id=14300099. [5] "Catalog of Living Whales". http://www.ubio.org/apps/Hershkovitz/ index.php?func=s&ID=42&t=s. Retrieved on 2008-10-01. [6] "Convention of International Trade in Endanged Species of Wild Flora and Fauna" (PDF). April 2000. http://www.cites.org/eng/cop/11/prop/ 14.pdf. Retrieved on 2008-10-01. [7] ^ "Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): Western North Atlantic Offshore Stock" (PDF). http://www.nmfs.noaa.gov/pr/pdfs/sars/ ao2003dobn-wnos.pdf. Retrieved on 2008-09-30. [8] "Tursiops truncatus: Species Information". IUCN. http://www.iucnredlist.org/search/ details.php?species=22563. Retrieved on 2006-11-03. [9] LeDuc R.G., Perrin W.F. and Dizon A.E. (1999). "Phylogenetic relationships among the delphinids cetaceans based on full cyctochrome b sequences". Marine Mammal Science 15: 619–648. doi:10.1111/ j.1748-7692.1999.tb00833.x. [10] Leduc, R., Perrin, W. & Dizon, E. (August 18, 1998). "Phylogenetic Relationships among the Delphinid Cetaceans Based on Full Cytochrome B Sequences". Marine Mammal Science 15 (3): 619–648. doi:10.1111/ j.1748-7692.1999.tb00833.x. http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/ journal/119937779/abstract. Retrieved on 2008-10-05. [11] Reeves, R.; Stewart, B.; Clapham, P.; Powell, J. (2002). Guide to Marine Mammals of the World. New York: A.A. Knopf. pp. 422. ISBN 0-375-41141-0. [12] "Risso’s Dolphin - American Cetacean Society". http://www.acsonline.org/ factpack/RissosDolphin.htm. Retrieved on 2008-09-20. [13] Lee, Jaennette (April 15, 2005). "WhaleDolphin Hybrid Has Baby ’Wholphin’". Associated Press. http://www.livescience.com/animals/ ap_050415_wolphin.html. Retrieved on 2008-09-20.

See also
• Cetacean intelligence • Dolphinarium • Audiograms in mammals

References
[1] Rice, Dale W (1998). Marine mammals of the world: systematics and distribution (Special Publication). Society of Marine Mammalogy. ISBN 1-891276-03-4. [2] ^ Shirihai, H. and Jarrett, B. (2006). Whales Dolphins and Other Marine Mammals of the World. Princeton: Princeton Univ. Press. p. 155–161. ISBN 0-691-12757-3. [3] Shirihai, H. and Jarrett, B. (2006). Whales Dolphins and Other Marine Mammals of the World. Princeton: Princeton Univ. Press. p. 159–161. ISBN 0-691-12757-3. [4] ^ Mead, James G. and Robert L. Brownell, Jr (November 16, 2005). Wilson, D. E., and Reeder, D. M. (eds).

9

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Bottlenose dolphin

[14] "The Mammals of Texas - Rough-toothed marine/dolphins/dolphins_botnose.html. Dolphin". http://www.nsrl.ttu.edu/tmot1/ Retrieved on 2008-09-16. stenbred.htm. Retrieved on 2008-09-20. [23] Thomson, Ainsley (2004-11-25). [15] "Robin’s Island Database about captive "Dolphins saved us from shark, Dolphins and Whales". lifeguards say". New Zealand Herald. http://www.robins-island.org/ http://www.nzherald.co.nz/section/1/ dolphins_database.php?filter=Sea_World_California. story.cfm?c_id=1&objectid=3613343. Retrieved on 2008-09-20. [24] "Dolphin rescues stranded whales". [16] Zornetzer H.R.; Duffield D.A. (October 1, CNN. 2008-03-12. http://edition.cnn.com/ 2003). "Captive-born bottlenose dolphin 2008/WORLD/asiapcf/03/12/ × common dolphin (Tursiops truncatus × nz.whales.ap/. Delphinus capensis) intergeneric [25] Brooks, David (2008-03-12). "Dolphin hybrids". Canadian Journal of Zoology rescues beached whales". Herald Sun. http://www.news.com.au/heraldsun/ (NRC Research Press) 81 (10): story/ 1755–1762. doi:10.1139/z03-150. 0,21985,23362100-5005961,00.html. http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/ nrc/cjz/2003/00000081/00000010/ [26] Read, Andrew (1999). ’Porpoises’. art00014?crawler=true. Stillwater, MN, USA: Voyageur Press. [17] Herzing, D., Moewe, K., & Brunnick, B. ISBN 0-89658-420-8. (2003). "Interspecies interactions [27] ^ Connor, R.; Wells, R.; Mann, J.; Read, between Atlantic spotted dolphins, A. (2000). "The Bottlenose Dolphin". in Stenella frontalis and bottlenose Mann, J.; Connor, R.; Tyack, P.; dolphins, Tursiops truncatus, on Great Whitehead, H.. Cetacean Societies. Bahama Bank, Bahamas" (PDF). Aquatic Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press. pp. 102. ISBN 0-226-50341-0. Mammals 29 (3): 335–341. doi:10.1578/ [28] Reeves, R.; Stewart, B.; Clapham, P.; 01675420360736505. Powell, J. (2002). Guide to Marine http://www.trustedpartner.com/docs/ Mammals of the World. New York: A.A. library/000059/ Herzing%20Moewe%20Brunnick%20final.pdf. Knopf. pp. 434. ISBN 0-375-41141-0. [29] "Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops Retrieved on 2008-09-20. truncatus): California/Oregon/ [18] Connor, Richards (2000). Cetacean Washington Offshore Stock" (PDF). societies: field studies of dolphins and http://www.nmfs.noaa.gov/pr/pdfs/sars/ whales.. Chicago: University of Chicago po2000dobn-cowos.pdf. Retrieved on Press. 2008-09-21. [19] Díaz López, Bruno; Shirai J.A. (2007). [30] Shirihai, H. and Jarrett, B. (2006). "Marine aquaculture and bottlenose Whales Dolphins and Other Marine dolphins’ (Tursiops truncatus) social Mammals of the World. Princeton: structure". Behavioural Ecology and Princeton Univ. Press. pp. 166. ISBN Sociobiology 62: 887. doi:10.1007/ 0-691-12757-3. s00265-007-0512-1. [31] ^ Wells, R. and Scott, M. (2002). [20] Díaz López, Bruno; Shirai J.A. (2008). "Bottlenose Dolphins". in Perrin, W.; "Marine aquaculture off Sardinia Island Wursig, B. and Thewissen, J.. (Italy):ecosystem effects evaluated Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. through a trophic mass-balance model.". Academic Press. p. 122–127. ISBN Ecological Modelling 212: 292-303.. 0-12-551340-2. doi:10.1016/j.ecolmodel.207.10.028 [32] "Dolphins - Skeletal Structure". (inactive 2009-04-11). http://library.thinkquest.org/17963/ [21] Díaz López, Bruno; Shirai J.A. (2006). skeletal-structure.html. Retrieved on "Bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) 2008-09-30. presence and incidental capture in a [33] "Bottlenose Dolphins - Physical marine fish farm on the north-eastern Characteristics". coast of Sardinia (Italy)". Journal of http://www.seaworld.org/infobooks/ Marine Biological Ass. UK 87: 113–117. Bottlenose/phychardol.html. Retrieved doi:10.1017/S0025315407054215. on 2008-09-30. [22] ^ "Bottlenose Dolphin at Animal Corner". http://www.animalcorner.co.uk/

10

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
[34] "Office of Protected Resources Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)". http://www.nmfs.noaa.gov/ pr/species/mammals/cetaceans/ bottlenosedolphin.htm. Retrieved on 2008-09-30. [35] "Comparative Mammalian Brain Collections - Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)". http://www.brainmuseum.org/ Specimens/cetacea/dolphin/index.html. Retrieved on 2008-09-30. [36] Díaz López, Bruno (2006). "Interaction between bottlenose dolphins and fisheries off Sardinia". ICES Journal of Marine Science 63: 946–951. doi:10.1016/j.icesjms.2005.06.012. [37] Au, Whitlow (1993). The Sonar of Dolphins. New York: Springer-Verlag. [38] Pack, A. A.; Herman L. M. (1995). "Sensory integration in the bottlenosed dolphin: Immediate recognition of complex shapes across the senses of echolocation and vision". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 98: 722–733. doi:10.1121/1.413566. [39] Herman, L. M.; Peacock, M. F., Yunker, M. P. & Madsen, C. (1975). "Bottlenosed dolphin: Double-slit pupil yields equivalent aerial and underwater diurnal acuity". Science 139: 650–652. doi:10.1126/science.1162351. PMID 1162351. [40] ^ "Dolphin Characteristics". http://www.wiu.edu/users/emp102/ DolphinWeb/dolphin_char.htm. Retrieved on 2008-08-31. [41] "Bottlenose Dolphins: Animal information, pictures, map". National Geographic. http://www.nationalgeographic.com/kids/ creature_feature/0108/dolphins2.html. Retrieved on 2006-11-03. [42] Janik, V. M.; Slater P. J. B. (1998). "Context-specific use suggests that bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls". Animal Behaviour 56: 829–838. doi:10.1006/anbe.1998.0881. [43] Herman, L. M.; Richards, D. G. & Wolz, J. P. (1984). "Comprehension of sentences by bottlenose dolphins". Cognition 16: 129–219. doi:10.1016/ 0010-0277(84)90003-9. [44] "Bottlenose dolphins". Seaworld. http://www.seaworld.org/animal-info/ info-books/bottlenose/physical-

Bottlenose dolphin
characteristics.htm. Retrieved on 2008-03-14. [45] ^ Kooyman, G. (2002). "Diving Physiology". in Perrin, W.; Wursig, B. and Thewissen, J.. Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. p. 339–341. ISBN 0-12-551340-2. [46] "Adaptations for an aquatic environment". Busch Gardens. http://www.buschgardens.org/infobooks/ Bottlenose/adapaqdol.html. Retrieved on 2008-03-14. [47] "Dolphin Q&A". Mia Research Foundation. http://www.monkeymiadolphins.org/ questions.htm. Retrieved on 2008-03-13. [48] "Do whales and dolphins sleep". howstuffworks. http://science.howstuffworks.com/ question643.htm. Retrieved on 2008-03-13. [49] "Dolphin Q&A". Mia Research Foundation. http://www.monkeymiadolphins.org/ questions.htm. Retrieved on 2008-03-13. [50] Atkinson, S. (2002). "Male Reproductive Systems". in Perrin, W.; Wursig, B. and Thewissen, J.. Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. pp. 700. ISBN 0-12-551340-2. [51] ^ Stewart, R. (2002). "Female Reproductive Systems". in Perrin, W.; Wursig, B. and Thewissen, J.. Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. p. 422-428. ISBN 0-12-551340-2. [52] ^ "Bottlenose Dolphins of Sarasota Bay". http://nationalzoo.si.edu/ ConservationAndScience/ AquaticEcosystems/Dolphins/ AboutDolphins/Reproduction.cfm. Retrieved on 2008-10-02. [53] Connor, Richard C., Peterson, Dawn M. 1994. The Lives of Whales and Dolphins. Henry Holt and Company: New York. [54] Connor, R.; Wells, R.; Mann, J.; Read, A. (2000). "The Bottlenose Dolphin". in Mann, J.; Connor, R.; Tyack, P.; Whitehead, H.. Cetacean Societies. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press. pp. 95. ISBN 0-226-50341-0. [55] Reeves, R.; Stewart, B.; Clapham, P.; Powell, J. (2002). National Audubon Society Guide to Marine Mammals of the World. New York: A.A. Knopf. p. 362-365. ISBN 0-375-41141-0.

11

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
[56] Mann, J. In press. Establishing Trust: Sociosexual behaviour and the development of male-male bonds among Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphin calves. In P. Vasey and V. Sommer (Eds.) Homosexual Behaviour in Animals: An Evolutionary Perspective. Cambridge University Press [57] "Spontaneous vocal mimicry and production by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): evidence for vocal learning". http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/ pubmed/8375147. Retrieved on 2008-08-31. is one example [58] "The Dolphin Institute - Behavioral Mimicry". http://www.dolphininstitute.org/our_research/ dolphin_research/ behavioralmimicry.htm. Retrieved on 2008-08-31. [59] Herman, L. (2002). "Language Learning". in Perrin, W.; Wursig, B. and Thewissen, J.. Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. p. 685-689. ISBN 0-12-551340-2. [60] "The Dolphin Institute - Understanding Language". http://www.dolphininstitute.org/our_research/ dolphin_research/ understandinglanguage.htm. Retrieved on 2008-09-31. [61] ^ Herman, L., Pack, A. & Wood, A. (August 26, 2006). "Bottlenose Dolphins Can Generalize Rules and Develop Abstract Concepts". Marine Mammal Science 10 (1): 70–80. doi:10.1111/ j.1748-7692.1994.tb00390.x. http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/ journal/119971600/ abstract?CRETRY=1&SRETRY=0. [62] "The Dolphin Institute - Awareness of One’s Own Recent Behaviors". http://www.dolphin-institute.org/ our_research/dolphin_research/ awarenessofownbehaviors.htm. Retrieved on 2008-08-31. [63] Herman, L. & Gordon, J. (January 1974). "Auditory delayed matching in the bottlenose dolphin1". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 21 (1): 19–26. doi:10.1901/jeab.1974.21-19. PMID 4204143. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/ articlerender.fcgi?artid=1333166. Retrieved on 2008-10-04.

Bottlenose dolphin
[64] "The Dolphin Institute - Awareness of One’s Own Body Parts". http://www.dolphin-institute.org/ our_research/dolphin_research/ awarenessofownbodyparts.htm. Retrieved on 2008-08-31. [65] "The Dolphin Institute - Pointing Gestures". http://www.dolphininstitute.org/our_research/ dolphin_research/pointinggestures.htm. Retrieved on 2008-08-31. [66] "Intelligence and Humans". http://www.wiu.edu/users/emp102/ DolphinWeb/dolphin_intel.html. Retrieved on 2008-08-11. [67] Marten, K. & Psarakos, S. (195). "Evidence of self-awareness in the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)". in Parker, S. T., Mitchell, R. & Boccia, M.. Self-awareness in Animals and Humans: Developmental Perspectives. Cambridge University Press. p. 361-379. http://earthtrust.org/delbook.html. Retrieved on 2008-10-04. [68] "Can Dolphins Count?". http://pr.sv.net/ aw/2007/January%202007/english/ pages010.htm. Retrieved on 2008-08-31. [69] Smolker, R.A., et al. (1997). "Spongecarrying by Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphins: Possible tool-use by a delphinid". Ethology 103: 454–465. [70] Krutzen M, Mann J, Heithaus MR, Connor RC, Bejder L, Sherwin WB (2005). "Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102 (25): 8939–8943. doi:10.1073/ pnas.0500232102. PMID 15947077. [71] Rendell, L.; Whitehead, H. (2001). "Culture in whales and dolphins". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (2): 309–382. doi:10.1017/ S0140525X0100396X. [72] Premack, D. & Hauser, M. (PDF). A whale of a tale: Calling it culture doesn’t help. pp. 1–3. http://www.wjh.harvard.edu/~mnkylab/ publications/learnconcepts/whale.pdf. Retrieved on 2008-09-30. [73] "Wild dolphins teaches others tail walking tricks". WDCS, the Whale and Dolphin Conservation Society. http://www.wdcs.org.au/ story_details.php?select=20.

12

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Bottlenose dolphin

[74] ^ Dicke, U. and Roth G. (August/ [84] Lavery, T. J., Kemper, C., Sanderson, K., September 2008), "Intelligence Evolved", Schultz, C. G., Coyle, P., Mitchell, J. G., Scientific American Mind: 75–77 Seuront, L. (2008). "Heavy metal toxicity [75] Heithaus, M. & Dill, L. (2002). "Food of kidney and bone tissues in South Availability and Tiger Shark Predation Australian bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops Risk Influence Bottlenose Dolphin aduncus)". Marine Environmental Habitat Use" (PDF). Ecology 83 (2): Research 67: 1. doi:10.1016/ 480–491. j.marenvres.2008.09.005. http://www.monkeymiadolphins.org/Pdf/ [85] Kenyon, P. (2004). BBC’s dining with the Heithaus%20and%20Dill%202002.pdf. dolphin hunters "Dining with the dolphin Retrieved on 2008-09-30. hunters". BBC. http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/ [76] Gannon, D., Barros, N., Nowacek, D., hi/programmes/this_world/3956355.stm Read, A., Waples, D. & Wells, R. (2004). BBC’s dining with the dolphin hunters. "Prey detection by bottlenose dolphins, Retrieved on 2008-09-30. Tursiops truncatus: an experimental test [86] "Bottlenose Dolphin Tursiops truncatus of the passive listening hypothesis". at MarineBio.org". http://marinebio.org/ species.asp?id=33. Retrieved on Animal Behavior 69 (3): 709. 2008-09-30. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.06.020. [87] ^ "The Dolphin Institute - Threats to the http://www.sciencedirect.com/ Bottlenose Dolphin and Other Marine science?_ob=ArticleURL&_udi=B6W9W-4FB9GWM-5&_user=10&_rdoc=1&_fmt=&_orig=search&_s Mammals". http://www.dolphinRetrieved on 2008-09-30. institute.org/resource_guide/ [77] Eisfeld, S. (PDF), The social affiliation conservation.htm. Retrieved on and group composition of bottlenose 2008-09-30. dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in the [88] Diaz Lopez, B., Shirai, J.A.; Bilbao Prieto, outer southern Moray Firth, NE A. & Méndez Fernández, P., 2008. Diving Scotland, p. 42, http://www.crru.org.uk/ activity of a solitary wild free ranging research/papers/eisfeld_thesis.pdf, bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). retrieved on 2008-09-30 Journal of Marine Biological Association [78] "Dolphin". The Columbia Encyclopaedia U.K.88(6): 1153-1157. (Sixth Edition ed.). 2006. [89] "Animal rights groups rap Pasig dolphin [79] Curran, S., Wilson, B. and Thompson, P show". http://newsinfo.inquirer.net/ (1996). "Recommendations for the topstories/topstories/ sustainable management of the view_article.php?article_id=105831. bottlenose dolphin population in the Retrieved on 2008-09-16. Moray Firth". Scottish Natural Heritage [90] "Katrina dolphin rescue launched". BBC. Review 56. 15 September 2005. [80] Kellyn S. Betts "Perfluoroalkyl Acids: http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/world/ What Is the Evidence Telling Us?" americas/4248178.stm. Retrieved on Environmental Health Perspectives 2008-02-14. Volume 115, Number 5, May 2007. [91] "Dolphins Deployed as Undersea Agents Accessed October 25, 2008. in Iraq". National Geographic. [81] Betts KS (July 2008). "Not immune to http://news.nationalgeographic.com/ PFOS effects?". Environ. Health news/2003/03/ Perspect. 116 (7): A290. PMID 0328_030328_wardolphins_2.html. 18629339. Retrieved on 2009-01-18. [82] Houde M, Martin JW, Letcher RJ, [92] Rehn, KW; Riggs, PK (2002). "Non-Lethal Solomon KR, Muir DC.: "Biological Swimmer Neutralization Study". U.S. monitoring of polyfluoroalkyl substances: Space and Naval Warfare Systems A review" Environ Sci Technol. 2006 Jun Center Technical Report Document 1;40(11):3463-73. Number 3138. http://archive.rubicon[83] Lavery, T. J., Butterfield, N., Kemper, C., foundation.org/7519. Retrieved on Reid, R. J., Sanderson, K: 2008. Metals 2008-09-25. and selenium in the liver and bone of [93] "U.S. Navy Marine Mammal Program three dolphin species from South Web Site". U.S. Navy Marine Mammal Australia, Science of the Total Program. http://www.spawar.navy.mil/ Environment 390(1): 77–85

13

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
sandiego/technology/mammals/ NMMP_FAQ.html. Retrieved on 2000-01-18. [94] "Naval Mine Warfare: Operational and Technical Challenges for Naval Forces". National Academies Press. 2001. http://books.nap.edu/ openbook.php?record_id=10176&page=70. Retrieved on 2008-10-02. [95] Dolphins and porspoises Australian Government: Dept. of the Environment, Water, Heritage and the Arts, Accessed Oct. 24, 2008 [96] "Bottlenose Dolphin". http://ourworld.compuserve.com/ homepages/jaap/tursiops.htm. Retrieved on 2008-08-11. [97] "American Cetacean Society - Bottlenose Dolphin". http://www.acsonline.org/ factpack/btlnose.htm. Retrieved on 2008-08-31.

Bottlenose dolphin
[98] "Flipper (1995)". IMDb. http://www.imdb.com/title/tt0111964/. Retrieved on 2006-11-03. [99] Constantine, R. (2002). "Folklore and Legends". in Perrin, W., Würsig, B. & Thewissen, J.. Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. pp. 448. ISBN 0-12-551340-2. General references • Hale, P.T., Barreto, A.S. and Ross, G.J.B (2000). "Comparative morphology and distribution of the aduncus and truncatus forms of bottlenose dolphin Tursiops in the Indian and Western Pacific Oceans". Aquatic Mammals 26 (2): 101–110. — Discusses distinguishing features between Bottlenose Dolphin species • Reiss, D. and Marino, L. (2001). Selfrecognition in the bottlenose dolphin: A case of cognitive convergence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, May 8, 98 (10), 5937-5942.

Retrieved from "http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Bottlenose_dolphin"

This page was last modified on 16 May 2009, at 23:16 (UTC). All text is available under the terms of the GNU Free Documentation License. (See Copyrights for details.) Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a U.S. registered 501(c)(3) taxdeductible nonprofit charity. Privacy policy About Wikipedia Disclaimers

14


								
To top