How brand perceptions affect purchase behavior

Document Sample
How brand perceptions affect purchase behavior Powered By Docstoc
					Giving the Wrong Impression? The Susceptibility of Brand Impressions

              to Associative Processes under Incidental Learning



       Instead of positioning a brand with a positive message, some companies choose to

use a double negative message. Normally a negative statement or imagery is shown first

and associated with the brand or its respective product category, and then the brand is

implied to be the opposite of that. The prevalence of double negative advertisements with

little accompanying support on its effectiveness should be investigated to determine how

consumers process such information. The questions of interest in this study are whether

viewers form the intended impression of brands and whether their impressions depend on

how they were formed (incidentally or intentionally).

       This study examines brand impression formation where ads are manipulated on

three factors: message type (positive and double negative), impression type (incidental and

intentional), persuasive appeal (affect- and cognitive-based). It is proposed that

impressions formed incidentally are more vulnerable to associative processes than those

formed intentionally since they involve more superficial processing (Carlston and Mae

2002). However, since impressions formed incidentally draw on superficial processing,

they are expected to be less persistent than those formed intentionally (Baddeley 1990). It

is also proposed that under incidental learning cognitive-based messages will result in less

accurate impressions than affect-based messages because they require more systematic

processing.
                                    INTRODUCTION

       A popular advertising method used to convey brand personality or positioning is to

explicitly describe or imply what a brand is not. For example, recent television

advertisements for Avis show what it would be like if the car rental company did not try

harder. The commercials feature long lines, inattentive employees, and unattractive car

rental facilities. In order for this message to be interpreted correctly, consumers must

engage in inferential processing to account for the context. With Avis, consumers must

understand that since Avis does try harder, they can expect exactly the opposite type of

service from that shown in the advertisement. Is it possible, however, that distracted or

unmotivated viewers do not make this connection and simply link the imagery shown in

the advertisement with the brand, thus associating Avis with incompetence?

       Marketing messages that utilize the double negative approach tend to follow a

similar sequence. They begin by presenting negative messages and imagery, often in the

form of an incompetent competitor, whether real or imagined, or a consumer having an

unpleasant purchase or consumption experience. Then the brand is disassociated from the

negative messages and imagery by implying or stating that the brand of interest and one’s

experience with it is the opposite of that depicted in the advertisement. The key

presumption is that consumers will form positive associations and impressions with the

brand of interest and negative associations with competing brands.

       This study attempts to show differences in impressions depending on the type of

marketing message (positive versus double negative) under incidental and intentional

impression formation. It is proposed that incidental impressions are formed more through

associative processes than intentional impressions, which would adversely affect




                                                                                            2
interpretation of double negative messages. In addition, the type of persuasive appeal may

influence how information is processed depending on whether it is primarily affect- or

cognitive-based. Research suggests affective and cognitive attitudes differ in complexity,

implying different persuasive appeals may require differing amounts of processing.

Therefore, the persuasive appeal will also be examined to determine if it interacts with

message type and impression type.

                                 LITERATURE REVIEW

       The crux of this study rests on previous work in impression formation, a topic that

has received considerable attention in the literatures of Social Psychology and Social

Cognition (e.g. Chen, Ybarra, and Kiefer 2004; Flynn 2005; Vazire and Gosling 2004).

The following section will define key concepts in impression formation, including how

impressions may be formed, to lay the foundation for this study. Attitudinal research on

attitude bases will also be discussed as it is proposed that different appeals may affect the

extent to which consumers process information. Finally, associative and inferential

processes will be reviewed as they are proposed to account for differences in brand

impression evaluations for positive and double negative advertisements.

Impression Formation

       Impression formation addresses how people make judgments about others,

specifically the attribution of personality traits to other individuals. However, impression

formation can also refer to how judgments are made about any object, including brands.

The impression formation paradigm is likely to be robust to brands because people tend to

think of and describe brands using human characteristics (Gilmore 1919). The brand

personality scale developed by Aaker (1997) possesses similar traits to those of the Big




                                                                                                3
Five, which provides support for similarity in how people describe brands and humans.

Since impression formation research involves the measurement of perceived personality

traits, the process should apply equally well to brands as it does to humans.

       The process of becoming aware of and forming impressions of brands can be

likened to one’s experience in meeting people. When meeting new others, one often tries

to control his or her appearance and behavior to convey a desired impression, much like

how marketers attempt to portray a brand in a certain light. However, impression

formation is not always a deliberate process as people are often too distracted or busy to

systematically process information about others (or brands) they encounter (e.g. Gilbert et

al. 1988; MacInnis, Moorman, and Jaworski 1991). Consequently, marketers may find

themselves surprised that particular marketing messages result in unexpected impressions

by consumers who incorporate peripheral cues and engage in associative processing.

       Consumers’ impressions of brands and their meaning may depend on how they

originally process information. Sometimes consumers are purposeful evaluators,

attempting to understand brands and learn what they stand for. At other times, their

learning of brands tends to be an accidental by-product of other goals where they lack

sufficient motivation and engagement to deliberately and purposefully process marketing

communications (Carlston and Mae 2003). Gilbert et al. (1988) suggest that the demands

of everyday life leave people “cognitively busy,” with their limited mental resources

focused on the most immediate demands of conversation and interaction with other people.

As a result, cognitive processes associated with superficial processing are often

incorporated into the impression formation process, resulting in rash and possibly

inaccurate impressions of brands.




                                                                                              4
Intentional and Incidental Impression Formation

       A formal distinction is made between intentional and incidental impression

formation. Intentional impression formation is the process of consciously and willfully

making inferences, attributions, or appraisals of a target object either during the processing

of stimulus information or later, during its recall. In contrast, incidental impression

formation is knowledge about an individual that is acquired inadvertently, during the

course of activities that are not directed at forming an impression (Carlston and Mae 2003).

It is proposed that most brand impressions are likely formed incidentally as consumers are

too busy to carefully process marketing messages or simply because they are overwhelmed

with seemingly endless advertising barrages (MacInnis, Moorman, and Jaworski 1991).

Consequently, since many impressions are not goal-oriented or purposeful, consumers

probably process information less intensively and more superficially.

       The effects of incidental exposure to marketing communications have been

thoroughly researched. Due to the sheer volume of advertising clutter and constraints on

our mind, researchers have found that our attention to and processing of ads is limited

(MacInnis, Moorman, and Jaworski 1991). Nonetheless, even incidental exposures to an

advertisement can increase the likelihood of the depicted brand being considered for

purchase even if consumers have no explicit memory of the ad (Shapiro, MacInnis, and

Heckler 1997). Research has also found that incidental exposure to advertisements

positively influences liking for brand names and product packages included in the

advertisement (Janiszewski 1990, 1993). The present study attempts to go beyond

showing effects of incidental learning and to show the differences in brand personality

attributions depending on whether impressions were formed incidentally or intentionally.




                                                                                             5
        There are obvious parallels between incidental and intentional impression

formation and incidental and intentional learning, dual process models (i.e. superficial vs.

systematic processing), implicit and explicit memory, and episodic and semantic memory.

A key assumption in this paper is that intentional impression formation is most often

systematic (i.e. involving attributional analyses of central, diagnostic cues, etc.), but that it

may sometimes be heuristic, relying on stereotypes or other peripheral cues. In contrast,

incidental impression processes are assumed to always be processed superficially (Carlston

and Mae 2003).

Persuasive Appeal and Attitude Base

        Attitudinal research often breaks attitudes down into affective and cognitive

components. Affective attitudes are evaluative responses about an attitude object resulting

from feelings and emotions, whereas cognitive attitudes are evaluative responses involving

thoughts and beliefs. Consumers’ attitudes toward most brands likely possess both

affective- and cognitive-based attitudes, although the relative strength of these attitude

bases may differ greatly. Research has demonstrated that consumers sometimes hold

predominantly affective attitudes for some brands and predominantly cognitive attitudes

for other brands (e.g. Millar and Millar 1990, Drolet and Aaker 2002). A significant

amount of research has examined how the persuasive appeal (affect- or cognitive-based) of

a marketing message affects persuasion depending on initial attitudes toward brands (e.g.

Edwards 1990). Studies have found support for the notion that affective and cognitive

attitudes differ in complexity or dimensionality (Edwards 1990, Zajonc 1980).

        Attitudes based on affect tend to have a more unidimensional structure organized

along a global evaluative dimension where specific attributes are either assimilated or




                                                                                                6
discounted (Edwards 1990). In contrast, cognitive attitudes have a multidimensional

structure based on a multitude of quality attributes. Support for this conclusion is found in

other studies that show affective attitudes are retrieved faster than cognitive attitudes

(Pham et al. 2001) and are better predictors of consumer choice than cognitive attitudes

under time constraints (Shiv and Fedorikhin 1999). Since cognitive attitudes have more

evaluative attributes than affective attitudes, it becomes more difficult to develop an appeal

targeted toward any specific cognitive dimension. Therefore, the increased complexity of

cognitive attitudes likely increases the amount of cognitive capacity needed to process

cognitive-based appeals. As a result, cognitive-based appeals likely require more

systematic processing than affective-based appeals.

Associative and Inferential Processes

       Marketers seek to create strong associations in consumers’ minds by linking

various cues and messages with their brands. As consumers pair these cues and messages

with brands, they may utilize different cognitive processes. Associative processes in a

marketing context involve consumers linking any cues shown in a marketing

communication with a brand while not taking into account the context or relation of the

cue to the brand. In contrast, inferential processes incorporate context and setting into

understanding the relationship between cues and brands.

       This distinction is important because of the popularity of double negative

advertisements which create negative cues and then disassociate them with the brand.

They require that consumers use inferential processing in order to correctly interpret the

message and form the desired impression. The Avis advertisement discussed earlier will

now be used to illustrate impression formation under associative and inferential processes.




                                                                                             7
Viewers processing the advertisement under associative processes will relate the imagery

shown (i.e. long lines and inattentive employees) and the brand in the commercial, but fail

to take the relationship of the two into account. Therefore, Avis would become associated

with incompetence under associative processing.

       Viewers processing this advertisement using inferential processes understand that

the poor car rental service illustrated in the commercial is a representation of what Avis’

service would be like if it did not try harder. And assuming they are aware that Avis’

motto and tagline is “We try harder”, the viewer understands that the actual service is

supposed to be the opposite of that displayed in the commercial. Consumers using

inferential processes should ultimately associate Avis with competence, and they may also

infer that other car rental companies are incompetent, presumably because they do not try

harder like Avis. Thus, a single message may be interpreted completely differently

depending on whether associative or inferential processes are employed.

                             THEORY AND HYPOTHESES

       It is proposed that incidental impressions are more likely to involve associative

processes because consumers do not purposefully process information as they do under

intentional impressions. The lack of cognitive effort put into the impression formation will

ignore situational indicators and context and simply pair cues with brands, regardless of

the supposed relationship between the two. It is expected that subjects that form incidental

impressions for double negative messages will ultimately come to associate the negative

aspects of the advertisement with the brand.

       H1: Incidental impressions are more likely to involve associative processes than
           intentional impressions




                                                                                              8
       Inferential processing requires more cognitive effort than associative processing.

Therefore, the ability to utilize inferential processing with cognitive-based appeals should

be even harder than for affect-based appeals because of the more complex,

multidimensional structure of cognitive-based attitudes. Regardless of how impressions

are formed, consumers are more likely to default to associative processes with cognitive-

based appeals compared to affect-based appeals because of cognitive overload.

       H2: Cognitive-based appeals are more likely to involve associative processes
           than affect-based appeals for both incidental and intentional impressions

       Understanding cognitive-based appeals should be harder under incidental

impression formation than under intentional impression formation. The lack of systematic

processing associated with incidental impressions will prevent consumers from using

inferential processing. In contrast, affective attitudes likely will not differ significantly

depending on impression type because of their more simple, unidimensional nature that

requires less systematic processing.

       H3: Incidental impressions for cognitive-based appeals will be more likely to
           involve associative processes than for affect-based appeals

       Persistence of impressions depends on the extent to which one engages in memory-

enhancing processes such as rehearsal or organization (Baddeley 1990). Since intentional

impressions are formed consciously, sometimes through extensive cognitive work, one

might expect these to be more accessible, more readily and often retrieved, and ultimately

more enduring (Carlston and Mae 2003). On the other hand, incidental impressions are

generally formed with less conscious thought and receive less processing. Therefore, it is

proposed that impressions formed incidentally are less persistent and less accessible.

       H4: Incidental impressions are less persistent than intentional impressions




                                                                                                9
                                      STUDY DESIGN

       The study will be conducted as a 2 x 2 x 2 mixed design experiment with the

following factors: impression type (incidental or intentional), message type (positive or

double negative), and persuasive appeal (affect- or cognitive-based). The dependent

variables will be personality traits relevant to each target advertisement. The experiment

will be carried out with undergraduate students that will receive course credit for a

marketing class.

       The target advertisements and the respective brands they represent will be designed

to convey certain personality traits. The first step in their generation will be to identify

unique personality traits for each ad concept to ensure that there is no cross contamination

from one target advertisement into another. Next, relevant product categories will be

chosen for each personality trait. For example, if “stable” or “strong” is chosen as a

personality trait, then a relevant product category where stability or strength is an

important attribute could be an insurance or financial services company. After the

personality traits and relevant product categories are selected and pretested to ensure

subjects perceive the traits are relevant to the product categories, basic ad concepts will be

created to emphasize the personality trait that is being conveyed.

       The next step will be to assign brand names to the products depicted in each ad

concept. Examining brand impression formation is much more difficult than impression

formation as applied to humans because of participants’ familiarity with and knowledge of

most brands. Namely, if participants are already familiar with brands in the study, they

already have preexisting impressions. Therefore, the brand names used for the




                                                                                               10
products/services in the study will have to be unknown and made up, and they will be

pretested to ensure they are relatively neutral.

       For each brand/ad concept, four different advertisements will be created, although

each subject will only see one of the versions for each brand. They will differ on message

type (positive and double negative) and persuasive appeal (affect- and cognitive-based),

but they will all convey the same personality trait. An affect- and cognitive-based ad will

be created both for a positive and nonnegative message. Using the Avis example with the

trait “competence”, a positive affect-based ad could show a customer being handed the

keys to a rental car with the tagline “Enjoy peace of mind with Avis” and a positive

cognitive-based ad could show the same scene but with the tagline “Ranked #1 in customer

satisfaction by J.D. Power & Associates”. As for double negative ads, an affect-based ad

could show a customer waiting in long lines with the tagline “Stressed out with

unprofessional service? Choose Avis” and a cognitive-based ad could show the same

scene but with the tagline “According to J.D. Power & Associates, other rental car

companies can’t compare to Avis’ customer satisfaction”.

       Participant’s impression type (incidental versus intentional) will be manipulated

based on their instructions for the experiment. Subjects will be told that an important

consideration in advertising is how people process advertisements that are buried in

magazines with a significant number of other ads and material. Consequently, they will be

told that the ads that they are to consider carefully are interspersed with other filler ads that

are not really relevant except to help provide context. Subjects will then be told that to

make things easier for them, they will be given a list of products about which they will be

asked, and these ads will be highlighted in some way when they appear. Subjects will be




                                                                                              11
directed to half of the actual target ads, and not to the other half, so that they process some

intentionally and some incidentally.

       Subjects will be exposed to 20 advertisements, 12 of which will be fillers and eight

of which will be brands of interest. There will be four main treatment groups, with each

group viewing only one version of each brand/ad concept. Each group will view four

positive target ads (two affect-based and two cognitive-based) and four double negative

ads (also two affect-based and two cognitive-based) for different brands. Participants will

view the ads as still images on a computer monitor. Each image will be shown for ten

seconds and will contain distinct imagery, persuasive appeals, and brand name.

       Since brands will be shown relatively briefly, the presentation will make it difficult

for participants to recall the specific imagery and taglines that were present when they

encountered each specific brand. To further ensure that their short-term memory is

flushed, participants will be asked to engage in an unrelated filler task following exposure

to the brands. Then participants will be shown the names of brands with imagery and

taglines deleted, and will then be asked to report their impression of each brand by rating

the brand on personality scales (Aaker 1997) theorized to be affected by the different ads

presented (e.g. Avis and competence), along with their liking of the brand. A memory

check will also be conducted to ensure subjects cannot make post-hoc impressions from

memory. Finally, in order to test the persistence of such impressions, this last step will be

repeated one week after the initial experiment.




                                                                                              12
                                 PROPOSED ANALYSIS

       Experimental data will be analyzed with repeated measures ANOVA with

personality traits and overall brand evaluations as the dependent variables. If associative

processes are present, it is expected that the negative aspect of the double negative

message will ultimately become associated with the brands. The effect of attitude base

will also be examined to determine if brand impressions depend on the type of persuasive

appeal of an ad, affect- or cognitive-based, and if this differs under message type (positive

and double negative) and on how impressions were formed (incidentally and intentionally).

       The same analysis will be repeated with the data taken a week after the experiment

to test the persistence of impressions formed incidentally and intentionally. The

participants’ recall of imagery and taglines associated with specific brands will be analyzed

as a manipulation check to make sure that they have minimal explicit memory of the

pairings.

                                       DISCUSSION

       Understanding impression formation and the differences between impressions

formed incidentally versus intentionally has important implications for branding.

Companies carefully position brands through advertising and by orchestrating the

marketing mix to achieve a desired positioning in the consumers’ mind. Nonetheless, great

variability may exist among consumers’ impression of brands based on how information

was originally processed. Companies must understand the context in which marketing

communications will be seen and prevent against misattributions from incidental

impressions.




                                                                                              13
       The pervasive use of negative imagery illustrating what a brand is not may

potentially backfire on a company. Consumers may ignore the relation between the

negative cues and the brand and ultimately associate the two together. Consequently,

significant effort to build a brand and create positive associations with it may potentially

have the opposite effect. Therefore, if companies insist on using these appeals, they must

ensure that consumers will be making impressions intentionally and not incidentally. The

consumers’ interest must somehow be piqued in order for intentional impression formation

to occur, possibly through striking images or incongruent information (Meyers-Levy and

Tybout 1989).

       The way in which consumers process information may also depend on whether it

was embedded in an affect- or cognitive-based message. Perhaps the double negative

message actually works with a particular attitude-based appeal due to the type of

processing involved. Another possibility is that different personality dimensions that

brands are trying to convey may be more associated with one attitude base over another.

Some dimensions may elicit very predominant affective attitudes, while other dimensions

will elicit primarily cognitive attitudes. The present research is designed with an eye

toward answering such critical questions.




                                                                                               14
                                      REFERENCES

Aaker, Jennifer (1997), “Dimensions of Brand Personality,” Journal of Marketing Research, 34
   (August), 347-57.

Baddeley, A. (1990), Human Memory. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Carlston, Donal and Lynda Mae (2003), “The Accidental Tourist,” in Foundations of social
   cognition: a festschrift in honor of Robert S. Wyer, Jr., Galen V. Bodenhausen and Alan J.
   Lambert, eds. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Chen, Serena, Ybarra, Oscar, and Amy K. Kiefer (2004), “Power and Impression
   Formation: The Effects of Power on the Desire for Morality and Competence,”
   Social Cognition, 22(4), 391-421.

Drolet, Aimee and Jennifer Aaker (2002), “Off-target? Changing Cognitive-based Attitudes,”
   Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12 (1), 59-68.

Edwards, Kari (1990), “The interplay of affect and cognition in attitude formation and
   change,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 202-16.

Fabrigar, Leandre R. and Richard E. Petty (1999), “The Role of the Affective and Cognitive
   Bases of Attitudes in Susceptibility to Affectively and Cognitively Based Persuasion,”
   Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25 (3), 363-81.

Flynn, Francis J. (2005), “Having an Open Mind: The Impact of Openness to Experience
   on Interracial Attitudes and Impression Formation,” Journal of Personality and
   Social Psychology, 88(5), 816-826.

Fournier, Susan (2002), Introducing New Coke, HBS No. 500-067.

Gilbert, D. T., B. W. Pelham, and D.S. Krull (1988), “On cognitive busyness: When person
    perceivers meet persons perceived,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
    733-40.

Gilmore, George W. (1919), Animism. Boston: Marshall Jones Company.

Janiszewski, Chris (1990), “The Influence of Print Advertisement Organization on Affect
    toward a Brand Name,” Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (June), 53-65.

Janiszewski, Chris (1993), “Preattentive Mere Exposure Effects,” Journal of Consumer
    Research, 20 (December), 376-92.

MacInnis, Deborah J., Christine Moorman, and Bernard J. Jaworski (1991), “Enhancing and
   Measuring Consumers’ Motivation, Opportunity, and Ability to Process Brand Information
   from Ads,” Journal of Marketing, 55 (October), 32-53.




                                                                                             15
Meyers-Levy, Joan and Alice M. Tybout (1989), “Schema congruity as a Basis for Product
  Evaluation,” Journal of Consumer Research, 16 (June), 39-54.

Millar, Murray G. and Karen U. Millar (1990), “Attitude change as a function of attitude type
   and argument type,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 217-28.

Pham, Michel T., Joel B. Cohen, John W. Pracejus, and David G. Hughes (2001), “Affect
   Monitoring and the Primacy of Feelings in Judgment,” Journal of Consumer
   Research, 28, 167-88.

Shapiro, Stewart, Deborah J. MacInnis, and Susan E. Heckler (1997), “The Effects of
   Incidental Ad Exposure on the Formation of Consideration Sets,” Journal of Consumer
   Research, 24 (June), 94-104.

Shiv, Baba and Alexander Fedorikhin (1999), “Heart and Mind in Conflict: The Interplay of
   Affect and Cognition in Consumer Decision Making,” Journal of Consumer Research, 26,
   278-92.

Vazire, Simine and Samuel D. Gosling (2004), “e-Perceptions: Personality Impressions
   Based on Personal Websites,” Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 87(1), 123-
   132.

Zajonc, Robert B. (1980), “Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences,”
   American Psychologist, 35, 151-175.




                                                                                            16

				
DOCUMENT INFO
Shared By:
Categories:
Tags:
Stats:
views:7
posted:7/30/2012
language:
pages:16